Sahara Fragile

Main Menu

  • Home
  • Sahara desert
  • Namib desert
  • Kalahari desert
  • Savanna desert
  • Fragile States

Sahara Fragile

Header Banner

Sahara Fragile

  • Home
  • Sahara desert
  • Namib desert
  • Kalahari desert
  • Savanna desert
  • Fragile States
Kalahari desert
Home›Kalahari desert›Liar, liar, wings on fire: this intelligent desert bird survives on cunning

Liar, liar, wings on fire: this intelligent desert bird survives on cunning

By Christopher J. Jones
May 1, 2014
0
0


[ad_1]

Once in Botswana, a southern African nation, I saw a troop of baboons making their way blindly through an overgrown field. The tops of their heads were barely above the tall grass, like the helmets of a military patrol ambushing. They were unaware of the presence of a cheetah cutting at an angle to cross them. I held my breath in anticipation. Then, all of a sudden, a bird – a small spoiler hornbill – gave a cry of alarm. Heads sprang up and the parties veered in opposite directions.

The idea that animals listen to alarm calls from other species is not new. Birds, mammals, and lizards do, and some species may even respond appropriately, depending on whether another species warns of an eagle or a snake. But a study published today in the journal Science shows a big step forward in this remarkable behavior with an account of the surprisingly complex “tactical deception” practiced by the fork-tailed drongo.

Related

A View to a Kill: these birds specialize in killing

Drongos are a common bird species across sub-Saharan Africa. They are shiny black and the size of an American robin. They have a reputation for being fearless and will easily flee from raptors many times their size. “If they see an eagle, they shoot straight up into the air like a surface-to-air missile and cling to the tail,” says Tom P. Flower, behavioral ecologist at the University of Cape Town and lead author. of the new study.

Drongos feed mainly on insects, often hawking them in the air, for example around outdoor electric lights. But they are also kleptoparasites: that is, they steal food that other species have located or captured. It’s a common way of living in the natural world, and lions, hyenas, jackals, and of course vultures do it too, but none with the art and daring of the fork-tailed drongo.

In the Kalahari Desert, a drongo will frequently travel in mixed packs with southern magpie chatterers. They also associate with meerkats. These other species benefit, according to the study by Flower and her co-authors, because drongos produce honest warning signals at any sign of a predator approaching. So having a drongo perched high on a nearby bush means chatterers and ground meerkats can spend less time being vigilant and more time foraging.

But their drongo watchdog is also lying. Because he has a habit of saving lives with his honest warnings, his false alarms also cause chatterers and meerkats to scatter, just long enough for the drongo to scurry around and steal their food.

You might think it would only work for a little while. We all know from the legend of the boy who cried wolf, that repeating the same lie over and over again causes everyone to ignore you, even if you mistake it for the occasional truth. So, in addition to their own alarm calls, drongos have learned to mimic the alarm calls of no less than 45 other species. They collect this spectacular repertoire mainly from other local birds and, of course, from chatterers and meerkats.

Drongos are by no means the only successful vocal imitators in the animal world, nor even the only ones to use mimicry as a means of deception. Burrowing owls in the American West, for example, mimic rattlesnakes to deter potential predators from entering their nests. But the drongos deploy their mimicry with an unprecedented degree of tactical variation. For example, chatterboxes predictably become just a little skeptical of drongo alarm calls, and they will start foraging again faster than they do after the alarm call of a different species. . Drongos produce impersonated calls 42% of the time, according to Flower. Talkers also become skeptical when they hear the same type of alarm three times in a row. So when a drongo makes repeated attempts to steal food from the same target, it uses a different alarm call three out of four times.

What’s remarkable is that drongos go to great lengths to get just 23% of their daily diet. But Flower stresses that this is no small advantage in such a harsh and resource-poor environment as the Kalahari, especially in winter. It may seem like the drongos took this game to such an inventive degree, at least in part for their own amusement. But the most likely reality is that becoming a smart thief is all about survival.

[ad_2]

Related posts:

  1. Operation Kalahari Desert claims another “victim”
  2. A thriving artists’ colony in the South African desert
  3. Teamwork in the Desert Explains the Global Model of Bird Cooperation
  4. Do you know about tourism in the desert? | New times
Tagskalahari desert

CATEGORIES

  • Fragile States
  • Kalahari desert
  • Namib desert
  • Sahara desert
  • Savanna desert

RECENT POSTS

  • In South Dakota and Nebraska Deep Red, voters used ballot initiatives to reduce inequality
  • Innovative solution for financial ills
  • “We were abandoned in the desert at 2 a.m.”: migrants expelled from Algeria to Niger
  • ILoveBoobies participates in Desert Dash
  • Land Bank Self-sufficiency is a mirage in the desert | News, Sports, Jobs

ARCHIVES

  • November 2022
  • October 2022
  • September 2022
  • August 2022
  • July 2022
  • June 2022
  • May 2022
  • April 2022
  • March 2022
  • February 2022
  • January 2022
  • December 2021
  • November 2021
  • October 2021
  • September 2021
  • August 2021
  • July 2021
  • June 2021
  • May 2021
  • April 2021
  • March 2021
  • February 2021
  • January 2021
  • December 2020
  • November 2020
  • October 2020
  • September 2020
  • August 2020
  • July 2020
  • June 2020
  • May 2020
  • April 2020
  • March 2020
  • February 2020
  • January 2020
  • December 2019
  • November 2019
  • October 2019
  • September 2019
  • August 2019
  • July 2019
  • June 2019
  • May 2019
  • April 2019
  • February 2019
  • January 2019
  • December 2018
  • October 2018
  • September 2018
  • August 2018
  • July 2018
  • June 2018
  • May 2018
  • April 2018
  • March 2018
  • February 2018
  • January 2018
  • December 2017
  • October 2017
  • September 2017
  • August 2017
  • June 2017
  • May 2017
  • March 2017
  • January 2017
  • November 2016
  • October 2016
  • September 2016
  • August 2016
  • June 2016
  • May 2016
  • April 2016
  • March 2016
  • January 2016
  • November 2015
  • July 2015
  • May 2015
  • April 2015
  • January 2015
  • December 2014
  • October 2014
  • September 2014
  • June 2014
  • May 2014
  • January 2014
  • November 2013
  • October 2013
  • September 2013
  • April 2013
  • March 2013
  • November 2012
  • May 2012
  • October 2011
  • November 2010
  • April 2008
  • July 2006
  • November 2005
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms and Conditions